Grammar
Tenses
Present
Present Simple
Present Continuous
Present Perfect
Present Perfect Continuous
Past
Past Simple
Past Continuous
Past Perfect
Past Perfect Continuous
Future
Future Simple
Future Continuous
Future Perfect
Future Perfect Continuous
Parts Of Speech
Nouns
Countable and uncountable nouns
Verbal nouns
Singular and Plural nouns
Proper nouns
Nouns gender
Nouns definition
Concrete nouns
Abstract nouns
Common nouns
Collective nouns
Definition Of Nouns
Verbs
Stative and dynamic verbs
Finite and nonfinite verbs
To be verbs
Transitive and intransitive verbs
Auxiliary verbs
Modal verbs
Regular and irregular verbs
Action verbs
Adverbs
Relative adverbs
Interrogative adverbs
Adverbs of time
Adverbs of place
Adverbs of reason
Adverbs of quantity
Adverbs of manner
Adverbs of frequency
Adverbs of affirmation
Adjectives
Quantitative adjective
Proper adjective
Possessive adjective
Numeral adjective
Interrogative adjective
Distributive adjective
Descriptive adjective
Demonstrative adjective
Pronouns
Subject pronoun
Relative pronoun
Reflexive pronoun
Reciprocal pronoun
Possessive pronoun
Personal pronoun
Interrogative pronoun
Indefinite pronoun
Emphatic pronoun
Distributive pronoun
Demonstrative pronoun
Pre Position
Preposition by function
Time preposition
Reason preposition
Possession preposition
Place preposition
Phrases preposition
Origin preposition
Measure preposition
Direction preposition
Contrast preposition
Agent preposition
Preposition by construction
Simple preposition
Phrase preposition
Double preposition
Compound preposition
Conjunctions
Subordinating conjunction
Correlative conjunction
Coordinating conjunction
Conjunctive adverbs
Interjections
Express calling interjection
Grammar Rules
Passive and Active
Preference
Requests and offers
wishes
Be used to
Some and any
Could have done
Describing people
Giving advices
Possession
Comparative and superlative
Giving Reason
Making Suggestions
Apologizing
Forming questions
Since and for
Directions
Obligation
Adverbials
invitation
Articles
Imaginary condition
Zero conditional
First conditional
Second conditional
Third conditional
Reported speech
Linguistics
Phonetics
Phonology
Linguistics fields
Syntax
Morphology
Semantics
pragmatics
History
Writing
Grammar
Phonetics and Phonology
Semiotics
Reading Comprehension
Elementary
Intermediate
Advanced
Teaching Methods
Teaching Strategies
Assessment
ANAPHOR RESOLUTION
المؤلف:
John Field
المصدر:
Psycholinguistics
الجزء والصفحة:
P12
2025-07-26
79
ANAPHOR RESOLUTION
An anaphor is a piece of language which refers back to a previously mentioned entity, action or idea. Examples are: personal or demonstrative pronouns (she, that), pro-verbs (did so), adverbs (there) and expressions like the latter. Anaphor resolution is the process that occurs when a listener or reader interprets an anaphor by linking it to its antecedent.
Anaphor resolution appears to be an automatic on-line process, but one that can slow down reading– especially where there is uncertainty as to what the antecedent is. Experiments using ambiguous anaphors provide insights into how an antecedent is chosen. Factors include:
Parallel position. A pronoun in subject position is taken to refer to an antecedent that is in subject position. Example: John phoned Bill. He said he was ill.
Current topic. A pronoun is taken to refer to a topic that has been thematised in the preceding sentence or is in current focus. Example: As for children, they don’t like them. Maybe they frighten them.
Proximity. A pronoun is taken to refer back to the most recent noun phrase. Example: He held some bread over the fire with a fork. It grew hot.
Sometimes these criteria (e.g. parallel position and proximity) may be in conflict. Further factors appear to be world knowledge, animacy and the thematic roles associated with a verb. The first of these accounts for the different resolutions of they in:
They bought the apples because they were cheap.
They bought the apples because they were hungry.
There has been much discussion of the kind of representation that an anaphor accesses. This is especially an issue in listening, where there is no opportunity to look back to check understanding. The listener has to carry forward a mental representation in which current topics are marked as likely subjects for anaphoric reference. One account represents items in the current mental representation as activated to various degrees, thus enabling an anaphoric link to be made to the most salient.
Those who include definite nouns within the class of anaphors point out that definiteness entails two different retrieval processes. With some noun phrases, the antecedent will be in explicit focus (see c. below), while with others it will not (see d.).
c. Erica unlocked the door with difficulty. The door tended to stick.
d. Erica unlocked the door with difficulty. The key tended to stick.
An important distinction is also made between surface anaphors which require an antecedent that is linguistically present and deep (or conceptual) anaphors which do not, as in:
e. The car needs washing. Don’t make promises– just do it.
In e, the listener/reader has to interpret the anaphor do it by reference to the entire meaning representation rather than to a specific entity held in focus within it.
Children learning to read find anaphor resolution problematic and the ability to resolve anaphors has been shown to be an important factor distinguishing skilled from less-skilled readers. The distance between antecedent and anaphor is an important factor: resolution appears to become more difficult as the memory load increases. However, this effect is observed to some degree in all young readers. What especially marks out the less-skilled reader seems to be a tendency to link anaphors to the nearest possible antecedent rather than to a developing mental representation of the whole text. They may even make links to real world phenomena outside the text (e.g. assuming that now refers to the moment of reading, not that of writing).
Language acquisition researchers working in the Chomskyan tradition have concerned themselves particularly with cataphora (sometimes termed backwards anaphora), where a pronoun refers forward to an entity which has not yet been mentioned. At a surprisingly early age, children are capable of correctly attributing a referent to the pronoun he in sentences such as While he was dancing, the Ninja Turtle ate pizza. They also distinguish this type of sentence from one such as He was dancing while the Ninja Turtle ate pizza, where he does not refer to an entity within the sentence. Another area of research has been the processing of reflexive pronouns. At an early age, children are much more successful in interpreting a sentence containing a reflexive (Cinderella’s sister points to herself) than a similar sentence containing an ordinary pronoun (Cinderella’s sister points to her).
See also: Syntactic parsing
Further reading: Garrod and Sanford (1994); Singer (1990); van den Broek (1994); Yuill and Oakhill (1992: Chap. 4)
الاكثر قراءة في Linguistics fields
اخر الاخبار
اخبار العتبة العباسية المقدسة

الآخبار الصحية
