Grammar
Tenses
Present
Present Simple
Present Continuous
Present Perfect
Present Perfect Continuous
Past
Past Continuous
Past Perfect
Past Perfect Continuous
Past Simple
Future
Future Simple
Future Continuous
Future Perfect
Future Perfect Continuous
Passive and Active
Parts Of Speech
Nouns
Countable and uncountable nouns
Verbal nouns
Singular and Plural nouns
Proper nouns
Nouns gender
Nouns definition
Concrete nouns
Abstract nouns
Common nouns
Collective nouns
Definition Of Nouns
Verbs
Stative and dynamic verbs
Finite and nonfinite verbs
To be verbs
Transitive and intransitive verbs
Auxiliary verbs
Modal verbs
Regular and irregular verbs
Action verbs
Adverbs
Relative adverbs
Interrogative adverbs
Adverbs of time
Adverbs of place
Adverbs of reason
Adverbs of quantity
Adverbs of manner
Adverbs of frequency
Adverbs of affirmation
Adjectives
Quantitative adjective
Proper adjective
Possessive adjective
Numeral adjective
Interrogative adjective
Distributive adjective
Descriptive adjective
Demonstrative adjective
Pronouns
Subject pronoun
Relative pronoun
Reflexive pronoun
Reciprocal pronoun
Possessive pronoun
Personal pronoun
Interrogative pronoun
Indefinite pronoun
Emphatic pronoun
Distributive pronoun
Demonstrative pronoun
Pre Position
Preposition by function
Time preposition
Reason preposition
Possession preposition
Place preposition
Phrases preposition
Origin preposition
Measure preposition
Direction preposition
Contrast preposition
Agent preposition
Preposition by construction
Simple preposition
Phrase preposition
Double preposition
Compound preposition
Conjunctions
Subordinating conjunction
Correlative conjunction
Coordinating conjunction
Conjunctive adverbs
Interjections
Express calling interjection
Grammar Rules
Preference
Requests and offers
wishes
Be used to
Some and any
Could have done
Describing people
Giving advices
Possession
Comparative and superlative
Giving Reason
Making Suggestions
Apologizing
Forming questions
Since and for
Directions
Obligation
Adverbials
invitation
Articles
Imaginary condition
Zero conditional
First conditional
Second conditional
Third conditional
Reported speech
Linguistics
Phonetics
Phonology
Semantics
Pragmatics
Linguistics fields
Syntax
Morphology
Semantics
pragmatics
History
Writing
Grammar
Phonetics and Phonology
Reading Comprehension
Elementary
Intermediate
Advanced
Identifying word sense
المؤلف: David Hornsby
المصدر: Linguistics A complete introduction
الجزء والصفحة: 184-9
2023-12-26
711
Identifying word sense
We can identify the sense of a word by examining its relations with other words, the most basic forms of which are antonymy, synonymy, hyponymy and hypernomy.
Following Saussure, we can identify both paradigmatic (or substitutional) relationships between lexemes, involving their interchangeability in a particular context, and syntagmatic ones, involving their collocational possibilities: for example, one may toast bread in English but grill meat, despite the fact that the activity involved – exposure to heat – is essentially the same. Many of the terms used by semanticists to describe these sense relations are familiar, but employed in a more precise or specialized sense.
Sense relations between lexemes can be determined by specifying the truth conditions of the sentences in which they occur, i.e. the set of conditions that must necessarily be met for a sentence to be declared true. Consider, for example, the following two statements:
The cat ate the starling.
The cat ate a bird.
The first is true if – and only if (for which one writes, conventionally, iff) – the second is true also. This is an implicational (or one-way) relationship of entailment, from which we can deduce that all starlings have the property of being birds. Entailments must hold true in all possible worlds, and not just in a particular set of contexts. One can possibly imagine a science-fiction novel being written in which, as a result perhaps of a bizarre radioactive accident, all starlings were green, or had four legs, but it is impossible to imagine starlings not being birds. In cases of entailment of this kind, we can say that starling is a hyponym of bird, and that bird is the superordinate term or hypernym of starling, robin, jackdaw, ostrich, penguin and so on.
In many cases, psycholinguistic evidence suggests that a superordinate term is associated in a speaker’s mind with a prototype, i.e. a typical member of the category in question. For the superordinate term bird, for example, English speakers are more likely to think of robins as being typical of the bird class than, say, ostriches or penguins.