Grammar
Tenses
Present
Present Simple
Present Continuous
Present Perfect
Present Perfect Continuous
Past
Past Continuous
Past Perfect
Past Perfect Continuous
Past Simple
Future
Future Simple
Future Continuous
Future Perfect
Future Perfect Continuous
Passive and Active
Parts Of Speech
Nouns
Countable and uncountable nouns
Verbal nouns
Singular and Plural nouns
Proper nouns
Nouns gender
Nouns definition
Concrete nouns
Abstract nouns
Common nouns
Collective nouns
Definition Of Nouns
Verbs
Stative and dynamic verbs
Finite and nonfinite verbs
To be verbs
Transitive and intransitive verbs
Auxiliary verbs
Modal verbs
Regular and irregular verbs
Action verbs
Adverbs
Relative adverbs
Interrogative adverbs
Adverbs of time
Adverbs of place
Adverbs of reason
Adverbs of quantity
Adverbs of manner
Adverbs of frequency
Adverbs of affirmation
Adjectives
Quantitative adjective
Proper adjective
Possessive adjective
Numeral adjective
Interrogative adjective
Distributive adjective
Descriptive adjective
Demonstrative adjective
Pronouns
Subject pronoun
Relative pronoun
Reflexive pronoun
Reciprocal pronoun
Possessive pronoun
Personal pronoun
Interrogative pronoun
Indefinite pronoun
Emphatic pronoun
Distributive pronoun
Demonstrative pronoun
Pre Position
Preposition by function
Time preposition
Reason preposition
Possession preposition
Place preposition
Phrases preposition
Origin preposition
Measure preposition
Direction preposition
Contrast preposition
Agent preposition
Preposition by construction
Simple preposition
Phrase preposition
Double preposition
Compound preposition
Conjunctions
Subordinating conjunction
Correlative conjunction
Coordinating conjunction
Conjunctive adverbs
Interjections
Express calling interjection
Grammar Rules
Preference
Requests and offers
wishes
Be used to
Some and any
Could have done
Describing people
Giving advices
Possession
Comparative and superlative
Giving Reason
Making Suggestions
Apologizing
Forming questions
Since and for
Directions
Obligation
Adverbials
invitation
Articles
Imaginary condition
Zero conditional
First conditional
Second conditional
Third conditional
Reported speech
Linguistics
Phonetics
Phonology
Semantics
Pragmatics
Linguistics fields
Syntax
Morphology
Semantics
pragmatics
History
Writing
Grammar
Phonetics and Phonology
Reading Comprehension
Elementary
Intermediate
Advanced
Stress assignment in English nouns: Pater (1995) OT model
المؤلف:
Ingo Plag
المصدر:
Morphological Productivity
الجزء والصفحة:
P168-C6
2025-02-04
363
Stress assignment in English nouns: Pater's (1995) OT model
I will briefly outline the general constraint hierarchy responsible for the stress patterns in English nouns, omitting a number of constraints not directly relevant for the present purposes. The specific phonological properties of -ize verbs result from the operation of constraints that occupy a lexically specified position in the general hierarchy. This is in line with the idea frequently expressed in the literature (e.g. Alber 1998, Benua 1995, 1997, Urbanczyk 1995, 1996) that members of derivational categories are subject to the general prosodic constraints of a language, but that these categories involve further faithfulness relations which enrich the general constraint hierarchy of the language. As we will see, such constraints do not only influence stress assignment, but also segmental stem alternations as those that can be observed with -ize derivatives.
The following of Pater's constraints are most important for the analysis of -ize verbs. They are also the ones that are highest in Pater's hierarchy. The constraints and their definitions are given in (1a), their hierarchical ordering is given in (1b):
(1)
Basically, it is these four constraints that are responsible for the well-known generalization that English nouns are stressed on the penultima if it is heavy, and on the antepenultimate syllable if the penultima is light, (e.g Giegerich 1992:187, Burzio 1994:43). Let us see how this works. The constraints NONFINALITY and R-ALIGN-HEAD are in direct competition with each other. If NONFINALITY is ranked above R-ALIGN-HEAD, this forces main stress, and the main stressed foot, off the final syllable. R-ALIGN-HEAD nevertheless forces the main stress to be as close as possible to the right edge of the word, with each syllable intervening between the head syllable and the edge counting as one violation. Thus the minimal violation of R-ALIGN-HEAD which satisfies NONFINALITY is main stress on the penult. Consider the stress assignment in agenda and Canada (from Pater 1995:17-18):
The optimal candidate involves only one R-ALIGN-HEAD violation, whereas the competing candidates either violate higher ranked NON-FINALITY or show an additional R-ALIGN-HEAD violation. Note that, obviously, PARSE-σ must be ranked lower than R-ALIGN-HEAD, because the optimal candidate has two unparsed syllables. TROCH rules out the iambic candidate (agén)da, whereas Ft-BlN has no effect with these candidates. Turning now to words with non-heavy penult, we see that stress must be placed on the penultima in order to satisfy either of the top-ranked constraints:
Candidates that have a light penultima cannot have penultimate stress because in that case they would either have a final prosodic head, as in Ca(náda), or non-binary foot, as in Ca(ná)da, or an iambic foot, as in (Caná)da. With either of these candidates, one of the top-ranked constraints is violated.
Let us now turn to -ize derivatives, which show the same kind of basic stress pattern as nouns, in that they never have ultimate primary stress.1 They thus markedly differ from other verbs, which usually carry their stress on the last syllable, especially if it is superheavy (e.g. Burzio 1994: chapter 3). Consider the following data:
(4)
The behavior of canonical verbs must be due to a different ranking of the constraints in (1a) above. Thus, it could be argued that with underived verbs NONFINALITY ranks below R-ALIGN-HEAD. The details of such an account still need to be worked out, but need not concern us here any further. The basic points are that nouns behave differently from verbs, that derived verbs behave differently from underived verbs and that -ize verbs behave in general like nouns, but with some requirements that are specific to this class of words.
Note that it is not only derived verbs that do not behave like their un-derived counterparts, but that also certain derived nouns and adjectives show an aberrant behavior. Thus, nominal derivatives in -ee and -eer, or adjectives in -esque pattern with canonical verbs in that they have ultimate primary stress. The reasons for this anti-canonical prosodic behavior of certain classes of derived words certainly merit further investigation, but in general we can say that suffixes may bring in their own requirements which may disturb the general pattern.
1 As mentioned already in note 13, Hiberno-English and many varieties of Caribbean English place ultimate primary stress on -ize derivatives. The analysis to be proposed here does not extend to these varieties.