Grammar
Tenses
Present
Present Simple
Present Continuous
Present Perfect
Present Perfect Continuous
Past
Past Continuous
Past Perfect
Past Perfect Continuous
Past Simple
Future
Future Simple
Future Continuous
Future Perfect
Future Perfect Continuous
Passive and Active
Parts Of Speech
Nouns
Countable and uncountable nouns
Verbal nouns
Singular and Plural nouns
Proper nouns
Nouns gender
Nouns definition
Concrete nouns
Abstract nouns
Common nouns
Collective nouns
Definition Of Nouns
Verbs
Stative and dynamic verbs
Finite and nonfinite verbs
To be verbs
Transitive and intransitive verbs
Auxiliary verbs
Modal verbs
Regular and irregular verbs
Action verbs
Adverbs
Relative adverbs
Interrogative adverbs
Adverbs of time
Adverbs of place
Adverbs of reason
Adverbs of quantity
Adverbs of manner
Adverbs of frequency
Adverbs of affirmation
Adjectives
Quantitative adjective
Proper adjective
Possessive adjective
Numeral adjective
Interrogative adjective
Distributive adjective
Descriptive adjective
Demonstrative adjective
Pronouns
Subject pronoun
Relative pronoun
Reflexive pronoun
Reciprocal pronoun
Possessive pronoun
Personal pronoun
Interrogative pronoun
Indefinite pronoun
Emphatic pronoun
Distributive pronoun
Demonstrative pronoun
Pre Position
Preposition by function
Time preposition
Reason preposition
Possession preposition
Place preposition
Phrases preposition
Origin preposition
Measure preposition
Direction preposition
Contrast preposition
Agent preposition
Preposition by construction
Simple preposition
Phrase preposition
Double preposition
Compound preposition
Conjunctions
Subordinating conjunction
Correlative conjunction
Coordinating conjunction
Conjunctive adverbs
Interjections
Express calling interjection
Grammar Rules
Preference
Requests and offers
wishes
Be used to
Some and any
Could have done
Describing people
Giving advices
Possession
Comparative and superlative
Giving Reason
Making Suggestions
Apologizing
Forming questions
Since and for
Directions
Obligation
Adverbials
invitation
Articles
Imaginary condition
Zero conditional
First conditional
Second conditional
Third conditional
Reported speech
Linguistics
Phonetics
Phonology
Semantics
Pragmatics
Linguistics fields
Syntax
Morphology
Semantics
pragmatics
History
Writing
Grammar
Phonetics and Phonology
Reading Comprehension
Elementary
Intermediate
Advanced
Conclusion: ‘productivity’ in syntax
المؤلف: Andrew Carstairs-McCarthy
المصدر: An Introduction To English Morphology
الجزء والصفحة: 97-8
2024-02-05
883
I hope to have made it clear both why productivity is a notion that must be approached cautiously, and how it is possible to untangle its various aspects. The most important findings to bear in mind are two. First, a process can be formally regular without being semantically regular, as is illustrated by the suffixation of -ion to produce nouns from verbs with the root -mit. Secondly, semantically regular relationships between lexemes (that is semantic relationships that have more or less widespread parallels involving other lexemes) can subsist without morphological support, as is illustrated by the terms for domestic animals at (2). If semantic and formal regularity often go together, that is hardly surprising, since lexemes so constructed will be relatively easy to learn and will provide the most natural models on which new lexemes can be created; but it is oversimplifying to classify as simply ‘irregular’ or ‘unproductive’ any morphological relationship that is not in all respects straightforward.
It is natural to ask why productivity crops up as an issue so insistently with word formation but not with sentence formation. Are there no syntactic constructions that are less productive than others? Such constructions do indeed seem to exist. For example, there is no obvious reason why the construction illustrated at (6), in which a verb has two objects, should be acceptable in those examples but unacceptable (or less readily acceptable) in the examples at (7):
(6) a. They gave us a present.
b. They faxed us the answer.
c. They allocated us two seats.
d. They baked us a cake.
(7) a. *They donated us some pictures.
b. *They yelled us the instructions.
c. *They planned us a holiday.
d. *They spoiled us the evening.
Seemingly, the lexical entries for at least some of these verbs must specify whether or not they tolerate the double-object construction. The reason why this sort of syntactic restriction is less usual than the kind of morphological restriction is not immediately obvious. It may simply be that the propensity for words (i.e. lexemes) to become lexical items, and thus to acquire idiosyncrasies, inevitably compromises the generality of the processes whereby complex words are formed (that is, processes of derivational morphology and compounding); on the other hand, the propensity for phrases to become lexical items is relatively weak. But why should this difference in propensity for lexical listing exist, given that wordhood is neither a necessary nor a sufficient condition for lexical-item status? A plausible answer is that shorter items are more likely to be lexically listed than longer items are, and words (even complex words) are generally shorter than phrases.