Grammar
Tenses
Present
Present Simple
Present Continuous
Present Perfect
Present Perfect Continuous
Past
Past Simple
Past Continuous
Past Perfect
Past Perfect Continuous
Future
Future Simple
Future Continuous
Future Perfect
Future Perfect Continuous
Parts Of Speech
Nouns
Countable and uncountable nouns
Verbal nouns
Singular and Plural nouns
Proper nouns
Nouns gender
Nouns definition
Concrete nouns
Abstract nouns
Common nouns
Collective nouns
Definition Of Nouns
Verbs
Stative and dynamic verbs
Finite and nonfinite verbs
To be verbs
Transitive and intransitive verbs
Auxiliary verbs
Modal verbs
Regular and irregular verbs
Action verbs
Adverbs
Relative adverbs
Interrogative adverbs
Adverbs of time
Adverbs of place
Adverbs of reason
Adverbs of quantity
Adverbs of manner
Adverbs of frequency
Adverbs of affirmation
Adjectives
Quantitative adjective
Proper adjective
Possessive adjective
Numeral adjective
Interrogative adjective
Distributive adjective
Descriptive adjective
Demonstrative adjective
Pronouns
Subject pronoun
Relative pronoun
Reflexive pronoun
Reciprocal pronoun
Possessive pronoun
Personal pronoun
Interrogative pronoun
Indefinite pronoun
Emphatic pronoun
Distributive pronoun
Demonstrative pronoun
Pre Position
Preposition by function
Time preposition
Reason preposition
Possession preposition
Place preposition
Phrases preposition
Origin preposition
Measure preposition
Direction preposition
Contrast preposition
Agent preposition
Preposition by construction
Simple preposition
Phrase preposition
Double preposition
Compound preposition
Conjunctions
Subordinating conjunction
Correlative conjunction
Coordinating conjunction
Conjunctive adverbs
Interjections
Express calling interjection
Grammar Rules
Passive and Active
Preference
Requests and offers
wishes
Be used to
Some and any
Could have done
Describing people
Giving advices
Possession
Comparative and superlative
Giving Reason
Making Suggestions
Apologizing
Forming questions
Since and for
Directions
Obligation
Adverbials
invitation
Articles
Imaginary condition
Zero conditional
First conditional
Second conditional
Third conditional
Reported speech
Linguistics
Phonetics
Phonology
Linguistics fields
Syntax
Morphology
Semantics
pragmatics
History
Writing
Grammar
Phonetics and Phonology
Semiotics
Reading Comprehension
Elementary
Intermediate
Advanced
Teaching Methods
Teaching Strategies
Assessment
SLIPS OF THE PEN (AND KEYBOARD)
المؤلف:
John Field
المصدر:
Psycholinguistics
الجزء والصفحة:
P267
2025-10-11
48
SLIPS OF THE PEN (AND KEYBOARD)
Small-scale errors of writing, which provide insights into the writing process– at motor level, at the level of lexical retrieval and even at planning level.
Motor errors result from a failure in the signal that the brain sends to the hand or in the contact between hand and keyboard. A striking feature of motor errors (e.g. the ! teh) is how recurrent they are in the output of some individuals. Typing is an activity that demands a great deal of conscious control at the outset, but that gradually becomes proceduralised into a set of automatic keystroke sequences– particularly for frequent words such as the. A characteristic of highly automatic procedures is that they are difficult to reverse, even though the wrong procedure may have been acquired. An interesting area of speculation is whether motor errors simply reflect the configuration of the keyboard (Tand E on the same line, H a line lower) or whether certain keystroke sequences are more difficult for the brain–hand partnership to acquire.
Motor errors in handwriting also show the effects of automatisation. When a string of letters is very frequent, the letters may be inadequately formed or may run into each other because of reduced attention and/or the greater speed of execution. One feature of rapid handwriting is the uncompleted letter, where, for example, the writer makes the upstroke of a b but fails to complete the letter and forms an l.
Sub-lexical errors. A writer might replace a string of letters with another that occurs frequently in other words. One explanation is that sequences of finger movements are chunked by the writer, and that, in signalling the word to be typed, the brain has selected the wrong chunk.
Example: details à detials, existence à existance.
Phonologically based errors. Around 20 per cent of slips of the keyboard involve the substitution of a word that sounds similar to the target one.
Examples are: thereà their, couldà good, you areà your, thanà that, tooà to.
Especially striking in Hotopf’s (1983) data is the substitution of 28 for 20A. Slips like these provide evidence of the part played by phonology in writing, with writers using ‘inner speech’ in order to store a clause while it is being written. It is much rarer to find words replaced by others that are similar in form but not pronunciation (e.g. there ! these).
Errors affecting function words. Function words are especially prone to typing errors. This might be a further reflection of the importance of phonology (the low salience of these words in speech). Or it might be that the writer processes function words at a lower level of attention than content words– perhaps as a result of their high frequency. Frequently one function word is substituted for another (for ! of); or a function word is duplicated (Saw the the movement).
Forward planning. A further type of Slip provides evidence of the way in which a writer plans ahead, storing words in a buffer ahead of executing them. The result is anticipatory errors such as difference intelligence tests or neighbourshoods. In other examples, a feature is transferred between two words, indicating that the two form part of the same stored chunk: e.g. using bothing approaches.
See also: Writing
Further reading: Fromkin (1980); Garman (1990: 230–6); Hotopf (1983)
الاكثر قراءة في Linguistics fields
اخر الاخبار
اخبار العتبة العباسية المقدسة

الآخبار الصحية
