Grammar
Tenses
Present
Present Simple
Present Continuous
Present Perfect
Present Perfect Continuous
Past
Past Simple
Past Continuous
Past Perfect
Past Perfect Continuous
Future
Future Simple
Future Continuous
Future Perfect
Future Perfect Continuous
Parts Of Speech
Nouns
Countable and uncountable nouns
Verbal nouns
Singular and Plural nouns
Proper nouns
Nouns gender
Nouns definition
Concrete nouns
Abstract nouns
Common nouns
Collective nouns
Definition Of Nouns
Verbs
Stative and dynamic verbs
Finite and nonfinite verbs
To be verbs
Transitive and intransitive verbs
Auxiliary verbs
Modal verbs
Regular and irregular verbs
Action verbs
Adverbs
Relative adverbs
Interrogative adverbs
Adverbs of time
Adverbs of place
Adverbs of reason
Adverbs of quantity
Adverbs of manner
Adverbs of frequency
Adverbs of affirmation
Adjectives
Quantitative adjective
Proper adjective
Possessive adjective
Numeral adjective
Interrogative adjective
Distributive adjective
Descriptive adjective
Demonstrative adjective
Pronouns
Subject pronoun
Relative pronoun
Reflexive pronoun
Reciprocal pronoun
Possessive pronoun
Personal pronoun
Interrogative pronoun
Indefinite pronoun
Emphatic pronoun
Distributive pronoun
Demonstrative pronoun
Pre Position
Preposition by function
Time preposition
Reason preposition
Possession preposition
Place preposition
Phrases preposition
Origin preposition
Measure preposition
Direction preposition
Contrast preposition
Agent preposition
Preposition by construction
Simple preposition
Phrase preposition
Double preposition
Compound preposition
Conjunctions
Subordinating conjunction
Correlative conjunction
Coordinating conjunction
Conjunctive adverbs
Interjections
Express calling interjection
Grammar Rules
Passive and Active
Preference
Requests and offers
wishes
Be used to
Some and any
Could have done
Describing people
Giving advices
Possession
Comparative and superlative
Giving Reason
Making Suggestions
Apologizing
Forming questions
Since and for
Directions
Obligation
Adverbials
invitation
Articles
Imaginary condition
Zero conditional
First conditional
Second conditional
Third conditional
Reported speech
Linguistics
Phonetics
Phonology
Linguistics fields
Syntax
Morphology
Semantics
pragmatics
History
Writing
Grammar
Phonetics and Phonology
Semiotics
Reading Comprehension
Elementary
Intermediate
Advanced
Teaching Methods
Teaching Strategies
Assessment
MEANING CONSTRUCTION
المؤلف:
John Field
المصدر:
Psycholinguistics
الجزء والصفحة:
P172
2025-09-14
25
MEANING CONSTRUCTION
Current theory rejects the idea that readers and listeners are passive and represents them as actively engaged in a task of meaning construction. An ‘effort after meaning’ impels them to impose an interpretation even on an obscure text, and to keep updating it as new information comes in. They do not ‘receive’ the message encoded by a speaker/writer; they have to reconstruct it from the material of the utterance.
Central to meaning construction is the distinction between: (a) the words on the page or in the ear; (b) the propositional information that a text contains (loosely, its literal meaning); and (c) the enriched and selective interpretation which a reader or listener takes away. In processing a text, a comprehender performs a number of operations. At sentence level, they:
extract propositional information;
make any necessary inferences;
enrich the interpretation by applying world knowledge;
integrate the new information into their mental representation of the text so far;
monitor their comprehension in case of misunderstanding.
At discourse level, they also have to:
recognise the hierarchical structure of the text;
recognise patterns of logic which link the parts of the text;
determine which parts of the text are important to the speaker/ writer or relevant to their own purposes.
A number of accounts of discourse comprehension attempt to describe the way in which text information is built into an overall meaning representation:
The early Kintsch and Van Dijk models (Van Dijk and Kintsch, 1983) feature higher-level units of meaning, termed macro-propositions, which are achieved through the reader making judgements about which items of information are central to the text. Meaning construction operates in three stages. There is a surface level which takes the form of the actual words used in the text. There is a text-base level at which propositional information is established on a sentence-by-sentence basis. Finally, there is a situational level which brings in external knowledge; it is at this stage that propositional information is transformed into macro propositions. Kintsch et al. attempted to support this theory experimentally by constructing texts based upon sets of propositions, which they categorised at several levels according to how critical they were deemed to be to the development of the text. They recorded that the more important propositions were more likely to be recalled.
The two-step Construction-Integration model (Kintsch, 1988) updates the earlier theories, relying more heavily on ‘bottom-up’ information from the text. At the construction stage, meanings are activated in the form of a loose network of associations. At the integration stage, a boost is given to information which is contextually relevant, so that a coherent text base can be created. Textual cues lead readers to give added weight to some sections: they might, for example, pay more heed to the opening sentence of a paragraph.
The theory of mental models (Johnson-Laird, 1983) also postulates two stages: one at which propositional information is available and one at which an enriched interpretation is achieved through the listener/ reader making inferences and bringing world knowledge to bear. A mental model is the representation of a text which they hold at any given moment. Whenever the latest proposition appears to make no reference to entities in the current model, the listener/reader initiates a new model. Similarly, two models are combined when the proposition seems to refer to entities that feature in both. Throughout, propositions are tested to ensure that their truth conforms to the truth of the model as a whole.
The Memory Focus Model (Sanford and Garrod, 1981) has developed from work on anaphor resolution. At any given moment, a reader has a model of a text, in which there is an explicit focus upon the elements of the text that are currently foregrounded and an implicit focus upon other information being carried forward. The processing of a text is selective and can be shallow or deep. In some circumstances, top-down notions of the purpose of the text may override information at local level. Hence the Moses illusion: asked the anomalous question How many animals of each kind did Moses put in the Ark?, many subjects answer ‘Two’.
The Structure Building Framework (Gernsbacher, 1990) also provides a model of how readers build a coherent representation of a text. Here, the first stage of comprehension consists of laying a foundation, which is why reading times are longer for the first word of a sentence or the first sentence of a paragraph. The reader maps incoming information on to a current information substructure if it coheres with what is there. If it does not, the reader employs a shifting process which involves creating a new information substructure. Less-skilled comprehenders are said to shift too often because they do not make the appropriate connections. As a result, they build meaning representations at a very local level and fail to construct more global ones.
See also: Focus, Mental model, Proposition, Schema theory, Story grammar
Further reading: Garnham (1985: 152–82); Gernsbacher and Foertsch (1999); Kintsch (1994: 729–36); Stevenson (1993: Chap. 5).
الاكثر قراءة في Linguistics fields
اخر الاخبار
اخبار العتبة العباسية المقدسة

الآخبار الصحية
