Grammar
Tenses
Present
Present Simple
Present Continuous
Present Perfect
Present Perfect Continuous
Past
Past Simple
Past Continuous
Past Perfect
Past Perfect Continuous
Future
Future Simple
Future Continuous
Future Perfect
Future Perfect Continuous
Parts Of Speech
Nouns
Countable and uncountable nouns
Verbal nouns
Singular and Plural nouns
Proper nouns
Nouns gender
Nouns definition
Concrete nouns
Abstract nouns
Common nouns
Collective nouns
Definition Of Nouns
Verbs
Stative and dynamic verbs
Finite and nonfinite verbs
To be verbs
Transitive and intransitive verbs
Auxiliary verbs
Modal verbs
Regular and irregular verbs
Action verbs
Adverbs
Relative adverbs
Interrogative adverbs
Adverbs of time
Adverbs of place
Adverbs of reason
Adverbs of quantity
Adverbs of manner
Adverbs of frequency
Adverbs of affirmation
Adjectives
Quantitative adjective
Proper adjective
Possessive adjective
Numeral adjective
Interrogative adjective
Distributive adjective
Descriptive adjective
Demonstrative adjective
Pronouns
Subject pronoun
Relative pronoun
Reflexive pronoun
Reciprocal pronoun
Possessive pronoun
Personal pronoun
Interrogative pronoun
Indefinite pronoun
Emphatic pronoun
Distributive pronoun
Demonstrative pronoun
Pre Position
Preposition by function
Time preposition
Reason preposition
Possession preposition
Place preposition
Phrases preposition
Origin preposition
Measure preposition
Direction preposition
Contrast preposition
Agent preposition
Preposition by construction
Simple preposition
Phrase preposition
Double preposition
Compound preposition
Conjunctions
Subordinating conjunction
Correlative conjunction
Coordinating conjunction
Conjunctive adverbs
Interjections
Express calling interjection
Grammar Rules
Passive and Active
Preference
Requests and offers
wishes
Be used to
Some and any
Could have done
Describing people
Giving advices
Possession
Comparative and superlative
Giving Reason
Making Suggestions
Apologizing
Forming questions
Since and for
Directions
Obligation
Adverbials
invitation
Articles
Imaginary condition
Zero conditional
First conditional
Second conditional
Third conditional
Reported speech
Linguistics
Phonetics
Phonology
Linguistics fields
Syntax
Morphology
Semantics
pragmatics
History
Writing
Grammar
Phonetics and Phonology
Semiotics
Reading Comprehension
Elementary
Intermediate
Advanced
Teaching Methods
Teaching Strategies
Assessment
LEXICAL STRESS
المؤلف:
John Field
المصدر:
Psycholinguistics
الجزء والصفحة:
P160
2025-09-10
79
LEXICAL STRESS
A phonological feature of many languages, where one syllable of a word carries greater prominence than the others. Lexical stress does not vary, and is assumed to be part of a word’s lexical entry. In English, lexical stress is marked acoustically by duration, loudness and/or pitch movement; and only falls on syllables with full quality vowels. In addition to the syllable bearing primary stress, others in a word may bear secondary stress– as in the third syllable of SUperMARket.
Stress appears to play an important part in the way speech is processed. There is evidence that listeners accord a higher level of attention to stressed syllables in English than to unstressed. This may be because stressed syllables are more prominent, especially in a noisy environment. They are also more reliable. They contain full quality vowels, and their phonemic segments, vowels and consonants alike, are less subject to reductions in duration. In addition, they are more informative. One way of representing imprecision in speech is to transcribe a corpus using a simplified system with only six phoneme categories. When researchers did this, they discovered that the number of words that were indistinguishable increased enormously if information relating to stress was not included.
One suggestion is that stressed syllables may provide the trigger for lexical access. POSS would form the access code for the word imPOSSible and TER for alternative. In this case, lexical access would not operate on strictly left-to-right principles, since access might be delayed until a stressed syllable was reached.
Listeners closely associate English weak syllables with functors. Indeed, it may be that unstressed syllables are not submitted to the lexicon at all, but are simply subjected to a pattern-matching process on the assumption that they constitute function words and have no lexical meaning.
A majority of the world’s languages (perhaps around 70 per cent) have lexical stress that consistently falls on the same syllable of a word. Where it always falls on the first or last syllable, it can be regarded as demarcative, enabling the listener to determine where word boundaries fall. About 50 per cent of languages appear to fit this profile.
Although English does not, stress patterns still seem to assist listeners to locate word boundaries. Subjects have proved quite accurate in dividing up pieces of ‘reiterant speech’ (utterances whose prosody is retained but whose syllables are replaced throughout bya sequence such as /mA:/). This may be because the majority of content words in English consist of a stressed monosyllable or have a stressed first syllable.
A difficulty in assessing the contribution of stress to speech processing is that stress is relative. A syllable that bears primary stress is more prominent in relation to the syllables around it. On this argument, some commentators have concluded that vowel quality is a better discriminator of syllable types since quality lends itself to clear and rapid binary distinctions (+ or - weak vowel).
See also: Lexical segmentation, SW (strong-weak) pattern
Further reading: Laver (1994)
الاكثر قراءة في Linguistics fields
اخر الاخبار
اخبار العتبة العباسية المقدسة

الآخبار الصحية
