Grammar
Tenses
Present
Present Simple
Present Continuous
Present Perfect
Present Perfect Continuous
Past
Past Continuous
Past Perfect
Past Perfect Continuous
Past Simple
Future
Future Simple
Future Continuous
Future Perfect
Future Perfect Continuous
Passive and Active
Parts Of Speech
Nouns
Countable and uncountable nouns
Verbal nouns
Singular and Plural nouns
Proper nouns
Nouns gender
Nouns definition
Concrete nouns
Abstract nouns
Common nouns
Collective nouns
Definition Of Nouns
Verbs
Stative and dynamic verbs
Finite and nonfinite verbs
To be verbs
Transitive and intransitive verbs
Auxiliary verbs
Modal verbs
Regular and irregular verbs
Action verbs
Adverbs
Relative adverbs
Interrogative adverbs
Adverbs of time
Adverbs of place
Adverbs of reason
Adverbs of quantity
Adverbs of manner
Adverbs of frequency
Adverbs of affirmation
Adjectives
Quantitative adjective
Proper adjective
Possessive adjective
Numeral adjective
Interrogative adjective
Distributive adjective
Descriptive adjective
Demonstrative adjective
Pronouns
Subject pronoun
Relative pronoun
Reflexive pronoun
Reciprocal pronoun
Possessive pronoun
Personal pronoun
Interrogative pronoun
Indefinite pronoun
Emphatic pronoun
Distributive pronoun
Demonstrative pronoun
Pre Position
Preposition by function
Time preposition
Reason preposition
Possession preposition
Place preposition
Phrases preposition
Origin preposition
Measure preposition
Direction preposition
Contrast preposition
Agent preposition
Preposition by construction
Simple preposition
Phrase preposition
Double preposition
Compound preposition
Conjunctions
Subordinating conjunction
Correlative conjunction
Coordinating conjunction
Conjunctive adverbs
Interjections
Express calling interjection
Grammar Rules
Preference
Requests and offers
wishes
Be used to
Some and any
Could have done
Describing people
Giving advices
Possession
Comparative and superlative
Giving Reason
Making Suggestions
Apologizing
Forming questions
Since and for
Directions
Obligation
Adverbials
invitation
Articles
Imaginary condition
Zero conditional
First conditional
Second conditional
Third conditional
Reported speech
Linguistics
Phonetics
Phonology
Semantics
Pragmatics
Linguistics fields
Syntax
Morphology
Semantics
pragmatics
History
Writing
Grammar
Phonetics and Phonology
Reading Comprehension
Elementary
Intermediate
Advanced
Underlying representations
المؤلف: David Hornsby
المصدر: Linguistics A complete introduction
الجزء والصفحة: 95-5
2023-12-16
670
Underlying representations
We have seen how the final [t] sounds of Rad and Rat in German can be understood as underlyingly ‘different’ although pronounced identically. In many other cases too, we can only account for the facts of what a native speaker ‘knows’ about his/her language by appealing to an underlying representation of the words he/she utters. While phonologists (and linguists generally) are sometimes accused of over-abstraction and/or abstruse terminology, there are occasions where explanation at an abstract level can yield valuable insights, as the following two examples will demonstrate.
Most varieties of British and southern hemisphere English are non-rhotic, i.e. they only have /r/ before vowels. Rhotic areas like North America, or the west of England, by contrast, do allow /r/ in other positions. The normal pronunciation of the following words in non-rhotic Essex, therefore, differs from that of rhotic Somerset (note that West Country accents use the retroflex /r/ variant ):
In spite of the fact that Essex speakers do not pronounce the /r/ in these words, many phonologists would argue that their underlying representations of each of these words is the same as that of their Somerset counterparts, i.e. that even for an Essex speaker car is underlyingly , but Essex speakers have an /r/ deletion rule which Somerset speakers do not. But does this not simply defy common sense? How can it be helpful to posit an underlying /r/ which simply isn’t there?
A phonologist might respond that, even for non-rhotic speakers, the /r/ actually is there, in some circumstances: even for non-rhotic speakers, it reappears before a vowel:
car /r/ of the year
Arthur /r/ and Jim
beer /r/ and sandwiches
Non-rhotic Essex speakers may not use the /r/ when these words are pronounced in isolation but they ‘know’ which words have an underlying /r/ which appears intervocalically, and which do not: no one would say tree */r/ on the hill for example. Not only does the abstract representation and /r/-deletion rule analysis account for a fact about the speaker’s knowledge of his/her language, it also captures a diachronic fact about English: non-prevocalic /r/ used to be pronounced in all varieties, but a change occurred in some, but not others, which led to its deletion. To account for these data otherwise would require specification of a complex /r/ rule for non-rhotic speakers setting out in detail the environments in which it can and cannot occur: positing an underlying /r/ in the representation looks altogether neater and more elegant. Some phonologists, it should be pointed out, would reject the underlying /r/ analysis in favor of an insertion rule on the grounds that many speakers insert the consonant even where it is etymologically unjustified, a phenomenon known as ‘intrusive r’:
law /r/ and order
India /r/ and Pakistan
Disputes between linguists are more likely to be over the most elegant account of the data available than about the data themselves!