Grammar
Tenses
Present
Present Simple
Present Continuous
Present Perfect
Present Perfect Continuous
Past
Past Continuous
Past Perfect
Past Perfect Continuous
Past Simple
Future
Future Simple
Future Continuous
Future Perfect
Future Perfect Continuous
Passive and Active
Parts Of Speech
Nouns
Countable and uncountable nouns
Verbal nouns
Singular and Plural nouns
Proper nouns
Nouns gender
Nouns definition
Concrete nouns
Abstract nouns
Common nouns
Collective nouns
Definition Of Nouns
Verbs
Stative and dynamic verbs
Finite and nonfinite verbs
To be verbs
Transitive and intransitive verbs
Auxiliary verbs
Modal verbs
Regular and irregular verbs
Action verbs
Adverbs
Relative adverbs
Interrogative adverbs
Adverbs of time
Adverbs of place
Adverbs of reason
Adverbs of quantity
Adverbs of manner
Adverbs of frequency
Adverbs of affirmation
Adjectives
Quantitative adjective
Proper adjective
Possessive adjective
Numeral adjective
Interrogative adjective
Distributive adjective
Descriptive adjective
Demonstrative adjective
Pronouns
Subject pronoun
Relative pronoun
Reflexive pronoun
Reciprocal pronoun
Possessive pronoun
Personal pronoun
Interrogative pronoun
Indefinite pronoun
Emphatic pronoun
Distributive pronoun
Demonstrative pronoun
Pre Position
Preposition by function
Time preposition
Reason preposition
Possession preposition
Place preposition
Phrases preposition
Origin preposition
Measure preposition
Direction preposition
Contrast preposition
Agent preposition
Preposition by construction
Simple preposition
Phrase preposition
Double preposition
Compound preposition
Conjunctions
Subordinating conjunction
Correlative conjunction
Coordinating conjunction
Conjunctive adverbs
Interjections
Express calling interjection
Grammar Rules
Preference
Requests and offers
wishes
Be used to
Some and any
Could have done
Describing people
Giving advices
Possession
Comparative and superlative
Giving Reason
Making Suggestions
Apologizing
Forming questions
Since and for
Directions
Obligation
Adverbials
invitation
Articles
Imaginary condition
Zero conditional
First conditional
Second conditional
Third conditional
Reported speech
Linguistics
Phonetics
Phonology
Semantics
Pragmatics
Linguistics fields
Syntax
Morphology
Semantics
pragmatics
History
Writing
Grammar
Phonetics and Phonology
Reading Comprehension
Elementary
Intermediate
Advanced
Synchronic arbitrariness and diachronic transparency The problem
المؤلف: APRIL McMAHON
المصدر: LEXICAL PHONOLOGY AND THE HISTORY OF ENGLISH
الجزء والصفحة: P264-C6
2024-12-31
115
Synchronic arbitrariness and diachronic transparency
The problem
There is one last apparent problem for [r]-Insertion, which I have not addressed so far, but which is frequently cited as the knock-down argument against it by proponents of alternative analyses: this involves apparent arbitrariness. Harris (1994: 246-7) succinctly expresses the issue in his claim that [r]-Insertion
is potentially arbitrary in two respects. First, the process itself must be considered arbitrary, unless grounds can be provided for assuming that it must be r that is inserted rather than any other randomly selectable sound ... There is no obvious local source in the surrounding vowels ... There is another respect in which R-Epenthesis is in need of justification. Why should it take place in the context it does, between vowels as long as the first is non-high? Would we have been surprised if it had applied in any other environment?
There have been attempts to establish a non-arbitrary conditioning context for [r]. Broadbent (1991) presents an element-based account of [r] in non-rhotic varieties, concentrating mainly on data from West York shire, where intrusive [r] is not stigmatized, and is therefore freely and productively produced, without the variable suppression found in RP. Broadbent observes that [r] is used as a hiatus breaker, compares it with the appearance of [j] and [w] in similar intervocalic contexts, and analyses all three as instantiations of a general process of glide formation. This is not strictly an insertion rule, but involves the spreading of some property of the preceding vowel to an adjacent empty onset to produce a surface glide: this gives [j] after high and mid long front vowels, [w] after high and mid long back vowels, and [r] after those other non-high vowels which can occur word-finally, namely [a: ɒ: ə ε з: ɒ] (1).
(1)
Because Broadbent's approach involves spreading rather than insertion, it can be incorporated into a declarative model. It also formalizes an insight a number of phonologists have used in accounting for the restricted distribution of intrusive [r], by invoking the characteristics of the preceding vowel. For instance, Johansson (1973) argues that /ɑ: ɔ:/ are schwa-final, and that schwa is the common factor which triggers [r]-Insertion (although this would not help with the rather different set of West Yorkshire trigger vowels), while McCarthy (1991) analyses all non-low long vowels as glide final, and claims that this glide blocks [r]-Insertion (although again this may not generalize to other varieties). Certainly, glide insertion and [r] must be related; we have already seen that [r] interacts with [j w] and [ʔ] formation, and also with other phonological processes such as [h]-dropping and vowel reduction.
Nonetheless, it seems that Broadbent's approach must be rejected. First, it makes wrong predictions; Broadbent (1991: 296) claims that `systems can have either linking and intrusive r or neither, but linking without intrusive r is not possible.' The data presented above speak against this, since we have seen that some varieties of Southern States USA and South African English do seem to have linking [r] only. It is also hard to see how Broadbent would account for the history of non-rhotic [r] if she predicts that no prior linking stage could have existed.
Secondly, Broadbent's account of spreading is incomplete: it is easy to establish what elements must be spreading to form [j] and [w] (I and U respectively, when these are the heads of preceding vowels), but much less straightforward for [r]. Broadbent attempts to isolate a common factor from the vowels after which [r] surfaces, and identifies the element A; but this is not the head of all relevant vowels. Consequently, although `the simplest assumption to make is that r-formation occurs when A is the head of a relevant segment', this `raises questions regarding the elemental composition of all non-high vowels, and clearly this requires further work' (Broadbent 1991: 299). In other words, Broadbent's account of [r] requires a wholesale reanalysis of the element structure of vowels, which she does not carry out. Finally, Broadbent (1991) gives no further details on how [r] results from A; if A spreads to an empty onset as an operator, as Broadbent assumes, this will produce schwa. In later work (Broadbent 1992), she argues that the correct output will arise if A can `pick up coronality' at some stage of the derivation; but there is no clear source for the coronal element.
Indeed, attempts to connect the features of /r/ to those of preceding, conditioning vowels may be doomed to failure, insofar as English /r/ itself covers so many variant realizations. Despite the best efforts of phoneticians (Lindau 1985) to identify an articulatory or auditory property unifying the class of rhotics, what makes an /r/ an /r/ also remains unclear cross-linguistically. Ladefoged and Maddieson (1996: 245) are forced to conclude that `Although there are several well-defined subsets of sounds (trills, ¯aps, etc.) that are included in the rhotic class, the overall unity of the group seems to rest mostly on the historical connections between these subgroups, and on the choice of the letter ``r'' to represent them all.'
It is hardly surprising that attempts to establish the affinities of /r/ with preceding vowels have proved fruitless, when we cannot even establish satisfactorily why /r/s form a natural class with themselves. This returns us to the accusation of arbitrariness. If anything, Broadbent's work makes matters even worse for [r]-Insertion, since her data from West Yorkshire indicate that the conditioning context may vary from dialect to dialect, and a random and variable set of vowels seems even harder to deal with than a random and fixed one. It is true that some of the West Yorkshire vowels are only realizational variants of the RP set, and that others are included because of the different phonotactic conditions operative in the different dialects; but nonetheless, the sets are distinct. How, then, are we to defend [r]-Insertion by demonstrating why [r] should be the segment inserted, and in these particular environments? Of course, this relates, as Harris points out, to the question of whether vowels affect the following /r/, or whether /r/ affects the preceding vowels. In fact, there is a single, historical solution to this composite problem of why and where [r] should be inserted.