Grammar
Tenses
Present
Present Simple
Present Continuous
Present Perfect
Present Perfect Continuous
Past
Past Continuous
Past Perfect
Past Perfect Continuous
Past Simple
Future
Future Simple
Future Continuous
Future Perfect
Future Perfect Continuous
Passive and Active
Parts Of Speech
Nouns
Countable and uncountable nouns
Verbal nouns
Singular and Plural nouns
Proper nouns
Nouns gender
Nouns definition
Concrete nouns
Abstract nouns
Common nouns
Collective nouns
Definition Of Nouns
Verbs
Stative and dynamic verbs
Finite and nonfinite verbs
To be verbs
Transitive and intransitive verbs
Auxiliary verbs
Modal verbs
Regular and irregular verbs
Action verbs
Adverbs
Relative adverbs
Interrogative adverbs
Adverbs of time
Adverbs of place
Adverbs of reason
Adverbs of quantity
Adverbs of manner
Adverbs of frequency
Adverbs of affirmation
Adjectives
Quantitative adjective
Proper adjective
Possessive adjective
Numeral adjective
Interrogative adjective
Distributive adjective
Descriptive adjective
Demonstrative adjective
Pronouns
Subject pronoun
Relative pronoun
Reflexive pronoun
Reciprocal pronoun
Possessive pronoun
Personal pronoun
Interrogative pronoun
Indefinite pronoun
Emphatic pronoun
Distributive pronoun
Demonstrative pronoun
Pre Position
Preposition by function
Time preposition
Reason preposition
Possession preposition
Place preposition
Phrases preposition
Origin preposition
Measure preposition
Direction preposition
Contrast preposition
Agent preposition
Preposition by construction
Simple preposition
Phrase preposition
Double preposition
Compound preposition
Conjunctions
Subordinating conjunction
Correlative conjunction
Coordinating conjunction
Conjunctive adverbs
Interjections
Express calling interjection
Grammar Rules
Preference
Requests and offers
wishes
Be used to
Some and any
Could have done
Describing people
Giving advices
Possession
Comparative and superlative
Giving Reason
Making Suggestions
Apologizing
Forming questions
Since and for
Directions
Obligation
Adverbials
invitation
Articles
Imaginary condition
Zero conditional
First conditional
Second conditional
Third conditional
Reported speech
Linguistics
Phonetics
Phonology
Semantics
Pragmatics
Linguistics fields
Syntax
Morphology
Semantics
pragmatics
History
Writing
Grammar
Phonetics and Phonology
Reading Comprehension
Elementary
Intermediate
Advanced
Vowels
المؤلف: APRIL McMAHON
المصدر: LEXICAL PHONOLOGY AND THE HISTORY OF ENGLISH
الجزء والصفحة: 146-4
2024-12-10
132
In (4.1), I reproduce the underlying vowel system for RP and GenAm which emerged from the emendations to Halle and Mohanan (1985) in chapters 2 and 3. An outline SSE/Scots system is listed in (4.2). Needless to say, each of these core systems contains a number of option points (often represented as bracketed vowels), and can be taken as temporary shorthand for a set of marginally different daughter systems; for a classification of Scots dialects using a similar system, see Catford (1958).
The two vowels bracketed in (4.2), /ø/ and /ε̈/, are very frequently encountered in Scots and occasionally in SSE. /ø/, a mid front rounded vowel, appears dialect-specifically in words like foot, floor, moon and spoon; it is the result of fronting of /o:/ to /ø:/ in Scotland, Northumberland, Cumberland, Durham, North Lancashire and Yorkshire in the late thirteenth or early fourteenth century. Aitken (1977) distinguishes three Modern Scots dialect-specific patterns of realization of earlier Scots /ù:/ (see (4.3)).
The length variation here results from the Scottish Vowel Length Rule, to be considered below. The quality differences seem to reflect variability in whether the /ø:/ vowel raised in the Great Vowel Shift, and the relative chronology of this raising, unrounding and SVLR.
/ε̈/ is more intriguing. Abercrombie (1979) notes that the first person to classify /ε̈/ as distinct from both /ɪ/ and /ε/ was A.J. Aitken, in whose honour it is sometimes called `Aitken's vowel'. It is easy to see why /ε̈/ evaded notice for so long, for various aspects of its quality, origin and distribution (both areal and lexical) remain opaque.
/ε̈/ characteristically occurs in words like bury, devil, earth, clever, jerk, eleven, heaven, next, shepherd, twenty, ever, every, never, seven, whether; however, Winston (1970), who tested a number of subjects from Edin burgh University for the presence and use of /ε̈/, found that although all her informants had contrastive /ε̈/, there was not one word where they all consistently used it. In addition, /ε̈/ has a regionally defined distribution, occurring principally in dialects of the West, the Borders, Perthshire and at least some parts of Edinburgh.
As for the quality of /ε̈/, it is generally negatively defined (Wells 1982: 404):
Where present, /ε̈/ is phonologically and phonetically distinct both from /ɪ/ and from /ε/, and in quality is typically somewhat less open than cardinal 3 and considerably centralized. The opposition can be tested by the triplet river vs. never vs. sever. If never rhymes neither with river (/ɪ/) nor with sever (/ε/), then it can be assumed to have /ε̈/.
Even less information can be gleaned on the origin of /ε̈/; perhaps the most plausible explanation is offered by Kohler (1964). Kohler notes that, in some Scots dialects, /ε̈/ is used in most of the words where SSE and RP have /ɪ/. He suggests that /ε̈/ was the original short vowel, but that /ɪ/ was later borrowed from English dialects and diffused variably through the same set of words. /ε̈/ tends to survive most consistently and widely in forms like never, shepherd, seven, where English dialects had /ε/ but Scots has /ε̈/ or native /ɪ/ - spellings like <niver> are relevant here.
There are a few other discrepancies between SSE and Scots dialects, some of which we shall return to below. For instance, whereas SSE has [u:] in two, Scots tends to have [e:], [a:] or [ɔ:], reflecting a pre-Great Vowel Shift monophthongization of word-final /ai/ in frequently used lexical items; the resulting /a:/ was then either raised in the Vowel Shift giving modern [e:], or was retained after a labial consonant, which in some areas rounded the vowel. Snow, blow have Scots [ɔ:] rather than SSE [o:], ultimately as a result of twelfth century Long Low Vowel Raising; differential operation of the same sound change in the north and south also gives the characteristic Scots [e] of stane, hame as opposed to RP and SSE stone, home. Finally, the diphthong /au/ is marginal in Scots, since the Great Vowel Shift failed for /u:/ > /au/ in the North, so that /u/ is retained in Scots house, out, cow. /au/ is present only in a few place names like Cowdenbeath, some specifically Scots lexical items like howff and loup, and words with earlier /ɔl/ 4 /ɔu/ 4 /au/ via l-Vocalization, as in gold [gΛud] and knoll [nΛu]. For details of these and other Scots sound changes, see Johnston (1997a).
Our main concern here, however, lies in the different patterns of distribution between SSE (and Scots), RP and GenAm vowels shown in (4.4).
There are several clear differences between the RP and GenAm systems on the one hand, and that of SSE on the other. Minor realizational discrepancies apart, these can be subsumed under one generalization. In RP and GenAm, all those vowels which can appear in stressed open syllables (that is, all vowels except /ɪ ε æ ɒ ʊ Λ ə/) surface consistently as long or diphthongal: Giegerich (1992) analyses these as alternative manifestations of underlying tenseness. A full discussion of the feature [± tense] must be deferred for the moment; let us accept for the moment that RP and GenAm are analyzable in terms of six pairs of vowels, the members of which are qualitatively and quantitatively distinct but of roughly the same height (4.5).
In SSE, this dual distinction of quantity and quality is not operative. Three of the oppositions are entirely lacking, with RP /ɑ/ ~ /æ/, /ɔ/~/ɒ/ and /u/ ~ /ʊ/ each replaced by a single vowel in SSE, conventionally represented as /a/, /ɔ/ and /u/ respectively. There are consequently a number of minimal pairs in RP which become homophonous for Scottish speakers, although Abercrombie (1979: 75±6) points out that more anglicized speakers of SSE may import these oppositions from RP. Typically, the introduction of /u/ ~ /ʊ/ presupposes /ɔ/ ~ /ɒ/, which in turn presupposes /ɑ/ ~ /æ/: the low vowel contrasts are quite common in SSE, especially in Edinburgh, but the /u/ ~ /ʊ/ distinction is very rare and tends to be inconsistently maintained.
The three remaining vowel pairs, /i/ ~ /ɪ, /e/ ~ /ε/ and /o/ ~ /Λ/, are relevant for Scots and SSE; but the nature of the opposition is different from that in RP and GenAm. Diphthongization in Scots dialects and in SSE is rare, even for the mid vowels /e/ and /o/, with the only surface diphthongs being realizations of the `true' diphthongs /Λɪ/ ~ /ai/, /au/ and /ɔi/. I have argued that these should be analyzed as underlyingly diphthongal, and this goes for Scots/SSE as well as RP and GenAm, although the exact nature of their underlying representations for Scots/ SSE will be the subject of some discussion below. Nor does length consistently distinguish the members of these pairs, since quantity in Scots and SSE is largely non-contrastive and context-sensitive; as shown in (4.4), /i e o/ are therefore long only in certain circumstances. The Scottish Vowel Length Rule (Aitken 1981, McMahon 1991, Carr 1992) is the process responsible, and a preliminary version is given in (4.6).
SVLR lengthens a certain set of vowels (to be defined later) when they precede /r/, any voiced fricative, or a bracket - the Lexical Phonological equivalent of a word or morpheme boundary. /i/ will consequently be long in beer, breathe, key, and keyed, but short in keep, wreath, keen and need. For the majority of vowels affected, SVLR simply controls an alternation of length, but for the diphthong /ai/, there is a concomitant change of quality with [Λi] in short and [a:i] in long environments.
We shall focus on SVLR. Evidence from previous discussions and formulations of the process will be used to ascertain precisely what subset of vowels constitutes its input, and what feature specification might characterize that class. Experimental investigations of SVLR will be assessed; these include work by Agutter (1988a, b), who claims that `the context-dependent vowel length encapsulated in SVLR is not, and perhaps never was Scots-specific' (Agutter 1988b: 20). The refutation of this assertion will involve a comparison of SVLR with a related lengthening process which operates in all dialects of English. However, before embarking on a detailed synchronic analysis, we must consider the historical source of SVLR: we shall see that a diachronic perspective is essential, here as in many other cases, to a full understanding of the present day situation.