Grammar
Tenses
Present
Present Simple
Present Continuous
Present Perfect
Present Perfect Continuous
Past
Past Continuous
Past Perfect
Past Perfect Continuous
Past Simple
Future
Future Simple
Future Continuous
Future Perfect
Future Perfect Continuous
Passive and Active
Parts Of Speech
Nouns
Countable and uncountable nouns
Verbal nouns
Singular and Plural nouns
Proper nouns
Nouns gender
Nouns definition
Concrete nouns
Abstract nouns
Common nouns
Collective nouns
Definition Of Nouns
Verbs
Stative and dynamic verbs
Finite and nonfinite verbs
To be verbs
Transitive and intransitive verbs
Auxiliary verbs
Modal verbs
Regular and irregular verbs
Action verbs
Adverbs
Relative adverbs
Interrogative adverbs
Adverbs of time
Adverbs of place
Adverbs of reason
Adverbs of quantity
Adverbs of manner
Adverbs of frequency
Adverbs of affirmation
Adjectives
Quantitative adjective
Proper adjective
Possessive adjective
Numeral adjective
Interrogative adjective
Distributive adjective
Descriptive adjective
Demonstrative adjective
Pronouns
Subject pronoun
Relative pronoun
Reflexive pronoun
Reciprocal pronoun
Possessive pronoun
Personal pronoun
Interrogative pronoun
Indefinite pronoun
Emphatic pronoun
Distributive pronoun
Demonstrative pronoun
Pre Position
Preposition by function
Time preposition
Reason preposition
Possession preposition
Place preposition
Phrases preposition
Origin preposition
Measure preposition
Direction preposition
Contrast preposition
Agent preposition
Preposition by construction
Simple preposition
Phrase preposition
Double preposition
Compound preposition
Conjunctions
Subordinating conjunction
Correlative conjunction
Coordinating conjunction
Conjunctive adverbs
Interjections
Express calling interjection
Grammar Rules
Preference
Requests and offers
wishes
Be used to
Some and any
Could have done
Describing people
Giving advices
Possession
Comparative and superlative
Giving Reason
Making Suggestions
Apologizing
Forming questions
Since and for
Directions
Obligation
Adverbials
invitation
Articles
Imaginary condition
Zero conditional
First conditional
Second conditional
Third conditional
Reported speech
Linguistics
Phonetics
Phonology
Semantics
Pragmatics
Linguistics fields
Syntax
Morphology
Semantics
pragmatics
History
Writing
Grammar
Phonetics and Phonology
Reading Comprehension
Elementary
Intermediate
Advanced
Why constraints? Halle and Mohanan (1985)
المؤلف: APRIL McMAHON
المصدر: LEXICAL PHONOLOGY AND THE HISTORY OF ENGLISH
الجزء والصفحة: 50-2
2024-11-27
167
The most extensive and comprehensive lexicalist account of English vowel phonology currently available is Halle and Mohanan (1985), and my attempts below to constrain the framework and mechanisms of LP will focus on this version of the theory. The critique developed here is applicable also to Mohanan (1986), which shares many of the assumptions of Halle and Mohanan (1985). Although Halle and Mohanan are working within the letter of LP, their analyses frequently fail to cohere with the spirit of the lexicalist enterprise, if we take that spirit to include the reduction of abstractness and the removal of unwarranted machinery and unmotivated derivation. I shall return in detail to the Halle and Mohanan model, considering many of their analyses in depth. For the moment, I intend only to demonstrate that their model cannot be seen as a significant advance over SGP.
First, let us turn to the general architecture of the model. As noted above, the Halle and Mohanan model comprises four lexical strata, as against Kiparsky's (1982) three and Booij and Rubach's (1987) two, as well as one postlexical level. If the number of levels proposed for a language is in principle unbounded, the theory loses any claim to explanatory adequacy, since an analysis would be possible in which each word-formation rule or phonological process were assigned to a separate stratum, or every rule to every stratum. Moreover, allowing random interspersal of cyclic and non-cyclic strata, as Halle and Mohanan also do, further compromises LP in that the operation or suspension of constraints will be purely a matter of stipulation. Nor do they strictly adhere to the four lexical strata they declare; their model also incorporates a so-called Loop, which allows compounded forms derived on their Level 3 to cycle back into Level 2 affixation. Since there is no principled limit to the levels between which loops could be proposed, the way would in principle be open for such morphological interaction between every pair of strata. In Halle and Mohanan's model, the concept of morphological level-ordering is effectively dead in the water.
There are similar problems of permissiveness in their specific phonological analyses. Halle and Mohanan are primarily concerned with General American, although they claim the underlying vowel system they propose (see (2.22)) is also appropriate for RP; this assignment of a single underlying phonological system to related dialects was a characteristic of SGP which Halle and Mohanan carry over into LP.
If we were looking for a predominantly surface-true vowel system, we would not find it here. The SPE account of Vowel Shift is retained, with the tense vowels shifting in sane, serene, divine, and free rides in parallel non-alternating plain, mean, pine. A single underlying representation is proposed for the vowel in balm, and one for bomb, although the realizations vary widely across British and American accents, and indeed the two are homophonous for many speakers. Absolute neutralization is rife, with [jū] in cube and [jū] in venue derived from distinct underlying vowels.
For the moment, I consider only one further example: the Modern English strong verbs. On the principle of `can do, will do' familiar from SPE, Halle and Mohanan elect to derive alternating pairs like seek ~ sought, choose ~ chose and blow ~ blew from single underlying forms. This requires, of course, the whole panoply of SGP derivational techniques, as illustrated in (2.23).
Connoisseurs of untrammelled generative analysis will find all their old favorite tricks here. We have non-surface-true underliers; special diacritic marking; a whole complex of ablaut and related rules which are required only for these strong verbs, and yet are presented as if equivalent to productive processes; abuses of extrinsic ordering involving `lay-by' procedures; and `Duke-of-York' derivations, which involve the production, destruction and re-derivation of the desired output, as in chose.
In the face of examples like these (and there are plenty to choose from), it must be clear that LP is in serious danger of losing sight of its founding aims. Lexical Phonologists are not, of course, alone in their weaknesses here; there is a recurring tendency in phonology to invent constraints or conditions, then see these as some panacea which means entirely unrestrained derivation is somehow all right. Take Brand X, drink anything you like, and you won't have a hangover. Except that 10-to-1 you still will, and Brand X will have wreaked its own havoc on your system in the meantime. And you still behaved like an idiot.
It should go without saying, but doesn't, that constraints are useless unless they are imposed and taken seriously. There is no point in formulating restrictions if we then devote all our intellectual energies to finding ways to defuse and disarm them; as we shall see, the use of underspecification to get round SP, and the ordering of rules on Level 2 to evade SCC are excellent examples of precisely this strategy. Identifying circumstances in which the constraints fail does not occasion congratulations. At best, it is a signal that we must find the underlying principle or factor which explains the non-application of the constraint. We shall see that such explanations may in fact be historical rather than synchronic.